Neural adaptation

Neural adaptation or sensory adaptation is a change over time in the responsiveness of the sensory system to a constant stimulus. It is usually experienced as a change in the stimulus. For example, if one rests one's hand on a table, one immediately feels the table's surface on one's skin. Within a few seconds, however, one ceases to feel the table's surface. The sensory neurons stimulated by the table's surface respond immediately, but then respond less and less until they may not respond at all; this is an example of neural adaptation. Neural adaptation is also thought to happen at a more central level such as the cortex.[1]

Fast and slow adaptation

One has to distinguish fast adaptation from slow adaptation. Fast adaptation occurs immediately after stimulus presentation i.e., within 100s of milliseconds. Slow adaptive processes that take minutes, hours or even days. The two classes of neural adaptation may rely on very different physiological mechanisms.

History

In the late 1800s, Hermann Helmholtz, a German physician and physicist, extensively researched conscious sensations and different types of perception . He defined sensations as the "raw elements" of conscious experience that required no learning, and perceptions as the meaningful interpretations derived from the senses. He studied the physical properties of the eye and vision, as well as acoustic sensation. In one of his classic experiments regarding how space perception could be altered by experience, participants wore glasses that distorted the visual field by several degrees to the right. Participants were asked to look at an object, close their eyes, and try to reach out and touch it. At first, the subjects reached for the object too far to the left, but after a few trials were able to correct themselves.

Helmholtz theorized that perceptual adaptation might result from a process he referred to as unconscious inference, where the mind unconsciously adopts certain rules in order to make sense of what is perceived of the world. An example of this phenomenon is when a ball appears to be getting smaller and smaller, the mind will then infer that the ball is moving away from them.

In the 1890s, psychologist George M. Stratton conducted experiments in which he tested the theory of perceptual adaptation. In one experiment, he wore a reversing glasses for 21½ hours over three days. After removing the glasses, "normal vision was restored instantaneously and without any disturbance in the natural appearance or position of objects."[2]

Modern version of inverting mirrors with harness.

On a later experiment, Stratton wore the glasses for eight whole days. By day four, the images seen through the instrument were still upside down. However, on day five, images appeared upright until he concentrated on them; then they became inverted again. By having to concentrate on his vision to turn it upside down again, especially when he knew images were hitting his retinas in the opposite orientation as normal, Stratton deduced his brain had adapted to the changes in vision.

Stratton also conducted experiments where he wore glasses that altered his visual field by 45°. His brain was able to adapt to the change and perceive the world as normal. Also, the field can be altered making the subject see the world upside down. But, as the brain adjusts to the change, the world appears "normal."[3][4]

In some extreme experiments, psychologists have tested to see if a pilot can fly a plane with altered vision. All of the pilots that were fitted with the goggles that altered their vision were able to safely navigate the aircraft with ease.[3]

Visual

Adaptation is considered to be the cause of perceptual phenomena like afterimages and the motion aftereffect. In the absence of fixational eye movements, visual perception may fade out or disappear due to neural adaptation. (See Adaptation (eye)).[5] When an observer's visual stream adapts to a single direction of real motion, imagined motion can be perceived at various speeds. If the imagined motion is in the same direction as that experienced during adaptation, imagined speed is slowed; when imagined motion is in the opposite direction, its speed is increased; when adaptation and imagined motions are orthogonal, imagined speed is unaffected.[6] Studies using magnetoencephalography (MEG) have demonstrated that subjects exposed to a repeated visual stimulus at brief intervals become attenuated to the stimulus in comparison to the initial stimulus. The results revealed that visual responses to the repeated compared with novel stimulus showed a significant reduction in both activation strength and peak latency but not in the duration of neural processing.[7]

Auditory

Auditory adaptation, as perceptual adaptation with other senses, is the process by which individuals adapt to sounds and noises. As research has shown, as time progresses, individuals tend to adapt to sounds and tend to distinguish them less frequently after a while. Sensory adaptation tends to blend sounds into one, variable sound, rather than having several separate sounds as a series. Moreover, after repeated perception, individuals tend to adapt to sounds to the point where they no longer consciously perceive it, or rather, "block it out". An individual that lives close to the train tracks, will eventually stop noticing the sounds of passing trains. Similarly, individuals living in larger cities no longer notice traffic sounds after a while. Moving to a completely different area, such as a quiet countryside, would then be aware of the silence, crickets, etc.[8]

Olfactory

Main article: Olfactory adaptation

Perceptual adaptation is a phenomenon that occurs for all of the senses, including smell and touch. An individual can adapt to a certain smell with time. Smokers, or individuals living with smokers, tend to stop noticing the smell of cigarettes after some time, whereas people not exposed to smoke on a regular basis will notice the smell instantly. The same phenomenon can be observed with other types of smell, such as perfume, flowers, etc. The human brain can distinguish smells that are unfamiliar to the individual, while adapting to those it is used to and no longer require to be consciously recognized.

Somatosensory

This phenomenon also applies to the sense of touch. An unfamiliar piece of clothing that was just put on will be noticed instantly; however, once it has been worn for a while, the mind will adapt to its texture and ignore the stimulus.[9]

Pain

While large mechanosensory neurons such as type I/group Aß display adaptation, smaller type IV/group C nociceptive neurons do not. As a result, pain does not usually subside rapidly but persists for long periods of time; in contrast, one quickly stops receiving touch or sensory information if surroundings remain constant.

Weight training

Studies have shown that there is neural adaptation after as little as one weight training session. Strength gains are experienced by subjects without any increased muscle size. Muscle surface recordings using electromyographic (SEMG) techniques have found that early strength gains throughout training are associated with increased amplitude in SEMG activity. These findings along with various other theories explain increases in strength without increases in muscle mass. Other theories for increases in strength relating to neural adaptation include: agonist-antagonist muscle decreased co-activation, motor unit synchronization, and motor unit increased firing rates.[10]

Neural adaptations contribute to changes in V-waves and Hoffmann's reflex. H-reflex can be used to assess the excitability of spinal α-motoneurons, whereas V-wave measures the magnitude of motor output from α-motoneurons. Studies showed that after a 14-week resistance training regime, subjects expressed V-wave amplitude increases of ~50% and H-reflex amplitude increases of ~20%.[11] This showed that neural adaptation accounts for changes to functional properties of the spinal cord circuitry in humans without affecting organization of the motor cortex.[12]

Habituation vs adaptation

The terms neural adaptation and habituation are often confused for one another. Habituation is a behavioral phenomenon while neural adaptation is a physiological phenomenon, although the two are not entirely separate. During habituation, one has some conscious control over whether one notices something to which one is becoming habituated. However, when it comes to neural adaptation, one has no conscious control over it. For example, if one has adapted to something (like an odor or perfume), one can not consciously force himself to smell that thing. Neural adaptation is tied very closely to stimulus intensity; as the intensity of a light increases, one's senses will adapt more strongly to it.[13] In comparison, habituation can vary depending on the stimulus. With a weak stimulus habituation can occur also immediately but with a strong stimulus the animal may not habituate at all[14] e.g. a cool breeze versus a fire alarm. Habituation also has a set of characteristics that must be met to be termed a habituation process.[15]

Rhythmic behaviors

Short-term adaptations

Short term neural adaptations occur in the body during rhythmic activities. One of the most common activities when these neural adaptations are constantly happening is walking.[16] As a person walks, the body constantly gathers information about the environment and the surroundings of the feet, and slightly adjusts the muscles in use according to the terrain. For example, walking uphill requires different muscles than walking on flat pavement. When the brain recognizes that the body is walking uphill, it makes neural adaptations that send more activity to muscles required for uphill walking. The rate of neural adaptation is affected by the area of the brain and by the similarity between sizes and shapes of previous stimuli.[17] Adaptations in the inferior temporal gyrus are very dependent on previous stimuli being of similar size, and somewhat dependent on previous stimuli being of a similar shape. Adaptations in the Prefrontal Cortex are less dependent on previous stimuli being of similar size and shape.

Long-term adaptations

Some rhythmic movements, such as respiratory movements, are essential for survival. Because these movements must be used over the course of the entire lifetime, it is important for them to function optimally. Neural adaptation has been observed in these movements in response to training or altered external conditions.[16] Animals have been shown to have reduced breathing rates in response to better fitness levels. Since breathing rates were not conscious changes made by the animal, it is presumed that neural adaptations occur for the body to maintain a slower breathing rate.

Transcranial magnetic stimulation

Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) is an important technique in modern cognitive neuropsychology that is used to investigate the perceptual and behavioral effects of temporary interference of neural processing. Studies have shown that when a subject’s visual cortex is disrupted by TMS, the subject views colorless flashes of light, or phosphenes.[18] When a subjects’ vision was subjected to the constant stimulus of a single color, neural adaptations occurred that made the subjects used to the color. Once this adaptation had occurred, TMS was used to disrupt the subjects’ visual cortex again, and the flashes of light viewed by the subject were the same color as the constant stimulus before the disruption.

Drug induced

Neural adaptation can occur for other than natural means. Antidepressant drugs, such as those that cause down regulation of β- adrenergic receptors, can cause rapid neural adaptations in the brain.[19] By creating a quick adaptation in the regulation of these receptors, it is possible for drugs to reduce the effects of stress on those taking the medication.

Post-injury

Studies in children with early childhood brain injuries have shown that neural adaptations slowly occur after the injury.[20] Children with early injuries to the linguistics, spatial cognition and affective development areas of the brain showed deficits in those areas as compared to those without injury. Due to neural adaptations, however, by early school-age, considerable development to those areas was observed.

See also

References

  1. Webster, Michael (1 November 2012). "Evolving concepts of sensory adaptation". F1000 Biology Reports. 4. doi:10.3410/B4-21. PMC 3501690Freely accessible. PMID 23189092.
  2. Stratton, George M. (1896). "Some preliminary experiments on vision without inversion of the retinal image" (PDF). Psychological Review. 3 (6): 611–7. doi:10.1037/h0072918.
  3. 1 2 Myers, David G. (2007). Exploring Psychology in Modules (7th ed.). New York: Worth Publishers. ISBN 978-1-4292-0589-4.
  4. Cullari, Salvatore (21 Mar 1997). "Re: Upside Down Glasses?". MadSci Network.
  5. Martinez-Conde, S; Macknik, SL; Hubel, DH (March 2004). "The role of fixational eye movements in visual perception." (PDF). Nature reviews. Neuroscience. 5 (3): 229–40. doi:10.1038/nrn1348. PMID 14976522.
  6. Gilden, D; Blake, R; Hurst, G (February 1995). "Neural adaptation of imaginary visual motion.". Cognitive Psychology. 28 (1): 1–16. doi:10.1006/cogp.1995.1001. PMID 7895467.
  7. Noguchi, Y; Inui, K; Kakigi, R (14 July 2004). "Temporal dynamics of neural adaptation effect in the human visual ventral stream.". The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 24 (28): 6283–90. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0655-04.2004. PMID 15254083.
  8. Anstis, Stuart (3 June 1985). "Re: Adaptation to auditory streaming of frequency-modulated tones". PsycNET.
  9. "Re: Sensory Adaptation: Definition, Examples & Quiz". Education Portal.
  10. Gabriel, DA; Kamen, G; Frost, G (2006). "Neural adaptations to resistive exercise: mechanisms and recommendations for training practices.". Sports medicine (Auckland, N.Z.). 36 (2): 133–49. doi:10.2165/00007256-200636020-00004. PMID 16464122.
  11. Aagaard, P; Simonsen, EB; Andersen, JL; Magnusson, P; Dyhre-Poulsen, P (June 2002). "Neural adaptation to resistance training: changes in evoked V-wave and H-reflex responses.". Journal of applied physiology (Bethesda, Md. : 1985). 92 (6): 2309–18. doi:10.1152/japplphysiol.01185.2001. PMID 12015341.
  12. Carroll, TJ; Riek, S; Carson, RG (15 October 2002). "The sites of neural adaptation induced by resistance training in humans.". The Journal of Physiology. 544 (Pt 2): 641–52. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.2002.024463. PMC 2290590Freely accessible. PMID 12381833.
  13. Sternberg, Robert (2009). Cognitive psychology (5th ed.). Australia: Cengage Learning/Wadsworth. pp. 137–138. ISBN 978-0-495-50629-4.
  14. Rakitin, A; Tomsic, D; Maldonado, H (September 1991). "Habituation and sensitization to an electrical shock in the crab Chasmagnathus. Effect of background illumination.". Physiology & Behavior. 50 (3): 477–87. doi:10.1016/0031-9384(91)90533-t. PMID 1800998.
  15. Thompson, RF; Spencer, WA (January 1966). "Habituation: a model phenomenon for the study of neuronal substrates of behavior." (PDF). Psychological Review. 73 (1): 16–43. doi:10.1037/h0022681. PMID 5324565.
  16. 1 2 Pearson, KG (2000). "Neural adaptation in the generation of rhythmic behavior.". Annual Review of Physiology. 62: 723–53. doi:10.1146/annurev.physiol.62.1.723. PMID 10845109.
  17. Verhoef, BE; Kayaert, G; Franko, E; Vangeneugden, J; Vogels, R (15 October 2008). "Stimulus similarity-contingent neural adaptation can be time and cortical area dependent.". The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 28 (42): 10631–40. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3333-08.2008. PMID 18923039.
  18. Silvanto, Juha; Muggleton, Neil G.; Cowey, Alan; Walsh, Vincent (2007). "Neural adaptation reveals state-dependent effects of transcranial magnetic stimulation". Eur. J. Neurosci. 25 (6): 1874–1881. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05440.x. PMID 17408427.
  19. Duncan, GE; Paul, IA; Harden, TK; Mueller, RA; Stumpf, WE; Breese, GR (August 1985). "Rapid down regulation of beta adrenergic receptors by combining antidepressant drugs with forced swim: a model of antidepressant-induced neural adaptation.". The Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics. 234 (2): 402–8. PMID 2991500.
  20. Stiles, J; Reilly, J; Paul, B; Moses, P (March 2005). "Cognitive development following early brain injury: evidence for neural adaptation.". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 9 (3): 136–43. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.01.002. PMID 15737822.
This article is issued from Wikipedia - version of the 9/27/2016. The text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution/Share Alike but additional terms may apply for the media files.